Chapter 5: Vision

Modified: 2025-08-24 1:11 PM CDST


In this chapter we begin our exploration of human and animal sensory systems. This chapter covers vision. To start, watch this video.

 


Module 5.1 Visual Coding (p. 147)

General Principles of Perception

The Eye and Its Connections to the Brain

Route Within the Retina—Bipolar Cells

Route Within the Retina— Amacrine Cells

Cross-Section of a Vertebrate Eye (p. 149)

Visual Path Within the Eye (p. 151)

The Optic Nerve

Illustration of the Blind Spot (p. 151)

Close your left eye and focus your right eye on the o in the top part. Move the page toward you and away, noticing what happens to the x. At a distance of about 10 inches (25 cm), the x disappears. Now repeat this procedure with the bottom part. At that same distance, what do you see?

The Fovea and the Periphery of the Retina

The Placement of Receptors on the Retina (p. 151)

One owlet has turned its head almost upside down to look up. Birds of prey have many receptors on the upper half of the retina, enabling them to see down in great detail during flight. But they see objects above themselves poorly, unless they turn their heads. Take another look at the prairie falcon at the start of this chapter. It is not a one-eyed bird; it is a bird that has tilted its head. Do you now understand why?

 

Convergence of Input onto Bipolar Cells (p. 152)

In the fovea, each bipolar cell receives excitation from just one cone (and inhibition from a few surrounding cones), and relays its information to a single midget ganglion cell. In the periphery, input from many rods converges onto each bipolar cell, resulting in higher sensitivity to faint light and low sensitivity to spatial location. Notice that the resolution is higher for the cones (smaller spots) and less for the rods (bigger spots). TV screens exploit the same principle: the more and smaller dots, the better.

 

The Difference Between Foveal and Peripheral Vision (p. 152)

Characteristic

Foveal vision

Peripheral vision

Receptors

Cones only

Proportion of rods increases toward periphery

Convergence of Input

Each ganglion cell excited by a single cone

Each ganglion cell excited by many receptors

Brightness sensitivity

Distinguishes among bright lights; responds poorly to dim light

Responds well to dim light; poor for distinguishing among

bright lights

Sensitivity

to detail

Good detail vision because each cone’s own ganglion cell sends a message to

the brain

Poor detail vision because many receptors converge their input onto a given ganglion cell

Color Vision

Good (many cones)

Poor (few cones)

 

Visual Receptors: Rods and Cones (p. 152)

Structure of Rods and Cones (p. 153)

(a) Diagram of a rod and a cone. (b) Photo of rods and a cone, produced with a scanning electron microscope. Magnification x 7000.

Photopigments

Color Vision (p. 153)

Visible Light on the Electromagnetic Spectrum (p. 154)

Specificity of Color Vision

Trichromatic Theory (p. 154)

Wavelength-Sensitivity Functions (p. 155)

Distribution of Cones in Two Human Retinas (from 13th ed.)

Investigators artificially colored these images of cones from two people’s retinas, indicating short-wavelength cones with blue, medium wavelength cones with green, and long-wavelength cones with red. Note the difference between the two people, the scarcity of short-wavelength cones, and the patchiness of the distributions.

The Opponent-Process Theory (p. 155)

An Afterimage as an Effect of Context (p. 156)

Stare at the x under bright light for a minute and then look at a white surface. Many people report an alternation between two afterimages, one of them based on the illusion of a red square.

Limitations of Color Vision Theories

The Context of Color Perception (p. 157)

After removal of the context, squares that appeared blue on the left or yellow on the right now appear gray.

Brightness Constancy (p. 157)

In the center of this figure, do you see a gray object above and a white object below? Place a finger over the border between them and then compare the objects.

Visual Constancies (read)

Stimulus Factors in Perception (read)

Color Vision Deficiency (p. 158)

Module 5.2 Visual Processing in the Brain (p. 161)

An Overview of the Mammalian Visual System (p. 161)

The Vertebrate Retina (p. 161)

The top of the figure is the back of the retina. The optic nerve fibers group together and exit through the back of the retina, in the “blind spot” of the eye.

The Path of Visual Input (p. 162)

a) one path to thalamus, another to superior colliculus, b) retinotropic organization of retinal axons.

Lateral Inhibition in the Retina

An Illustration of Lateral Inhibition (p. 163)

Try to "catch" the dots that appear in the periphery of your visual field. You cannot.

Processing in the Retina (p. 162)

Further Processing (p. 163)

Receptive Fields (p. 164)

Primate Receptive Fields

Parvocellular Neurons

Magnocellular Neurons

Koniocellular Neurons

Characteristics of Receptive Fields

The Primary Visual Cortex (1 of 2)

Simple Cells

Complex Cells

Simple and Complex Receptive Fields (p. 166)

The Receptive Field of a Complex Cell (p. 168)

End-Stopped or Hypercomplex Cells

The Receptive Field of an End-Stopped Cell (p. 167)

The cell responds to a bar in a particular orientation (in this case horizontal) anywhere in its receptive field, provided the bar does not extend into a strongly inhibitory area.

Cells in the Primary Visual Cortex (p. 168)

Characteristic

Simple Cells

Complex Cells

End-Stopped Cells

Location

V1

V1 andV2

V1 and V2

Binocular input

Yes

Yes

Yes

Size of receptive field

Smallest

Medium

Largest

Receptive field

Bar- or edge-shaped, with fixed excitatory and inhibitory zones

Bat- or edge-shaped, but responding equally throughout a large receptive field

Same as complex cell but with a strong inhibitory zone at one end

 

Columnar Organization of the Visual Cortex

 

When an electrode passes perpendicular to the surface of the cortex (first part of line A), it encounters a sequence of neurons responsive to the same orientation of a stimulus. (The colored lines show the preferred stimulus orientation for each cell.) When an electrode passes across columns (B, or second part of A), it encounters neurons responsive to different orientations. Column borders are drawn hereto illustrate the point; no such borders are visible in the real cortex.

Are Visual Cortex Cells Feature Detectors? (p. 169)

 

Development of the Visual Cortex (p. 169)

Critical Periods in Development

Stereoscopic Depth Perception

Strabismus

Strabismus Examples (p. 170)

Early Exposure to a Limited Array of Patterns (p. 170)

Restricting Early Visual Experience (p. 171)

For a few hours a day, the kitten wears goggles that show just one stimulus, such as horizontal stripes or diagonal stripes. For the rest of the day, the kitten stays with its mother in a dark room without the mask.

An Informal Test for Astigmatism (p. 171)

Do the lines in one direction look darker or sharper than the other lines do? If you wear corrective lenses, try this demonstration both with and without your lenses. (My implanted lenses correct for a great deal of astigmatism in each eye and each eye has its own, unique pattern.)

 

Impaired Infant Vision and Long-Term Consequences (p. 171)

Module 5.3 Specialized Visual Processes (p. 176)

The Ventral and Dorsal Streams

Shape Perception (p. 177)

Transformations of a Drawing (p. 177)

In the inferior temporal cortex, cells that respond strongly to the original respond about the same to the contrast reversal and mirror image but not to the figure–ground reversal. Note that the figure–ground reversal resembles the original in terms of the pattern of light and darkness, but it is not perceived as the same object.

Visual Agnosia

Face Recognition—The Fusiform Gyrus (p. 178)

During facial recognition many cells in fusiform gyrus are active.

Recognizing Faces

Amount of Time Infants Spend Looking at Patterns (p. 179)

How Infants Divided Their Attention Between Faces (p. 180)

A right-side-up face drew more attention than an upside-down one, regardless of whether the faces were realistic (left pair) or distorted (central pair). They divided their attention about equally between two right-side-up faces (right pair), even though one was realistic and the other was distorted.

Prosopagnosia

Motion Perception (p. 180)

Stimuli That Excite the Dorsal Part of Area MST (p. 181)

Cells here respond if a whole scene expands, contracts, or rotates. That is, they respond if the observer moves forward or backward or tilts his or her head.

Motion Blindness

Saccades

Illusions (not in text)

Back to Main Page