Edward Lee Thorndike (1874-1949)
Thorndike
was born in Lowell, Massachusetts. His father was a Methodist minister who ran
a strictly religious household. Thorndike attended Wellesley College where he
promptly left his religious upbringing behind. While there he read JamesÕ Principles of Psychology and was
inspired to enroll at Harvard to study psychology. MŸnsterberg
was back in Germany when Thorndike arrived at Harvard, so he took his courses
from James. He also began the animal research that was to make him famous.
Originally, he had planned to conduct research on children, but was unable to
get his project approved. So, he decided to look at maze learning in newly
hatched chicks. He was inspired by the earlier observations of Romanes and Morgan (see chapter 7). Thorndike wanted to
explore learning more thoroughly and to do so experimentally. He had to conduct
the research with chicks in the basement of William JamesÕ house because his
landlady would not allow him to keep chicks in his apartment. The results he
obtained were promising; his chicks did seem to run his simple mazes
(constructed out of old textbooks) more quickly with experience. He was living
hand to mouth at Harvard, however, so when Cattell
offered him a fellowship at Columbia he moved there taking a few of his best
chicks with him.
Border with Biology: Comparative
Psychology-Before Thorndike most behavioral research with animals
had been observational. Thorndike expressly set out to experimentally confirm
some of the observations previously published by Romanes
and Morgan. Although Thorndike essentially abandoned comparative psychology
afterward, he nevertheless inspired a new generation of animal researchers,
including John B. Watson.
He
finally secured laboratory space at Columbia in the attic of Schermerhorn Hall, then and now the home of the psychology
department. In that attic he began a new series of experiments designed to look
more carefully at the problem of learning. He built a series of puzzle boxes
from which either cats or dogs could only escape by learning to operate the
mechanism. Some boxes required the animal to step on a treadle, others required
them to pull on a loop, and still others required a sequence of steps. He kept
his animals hungry and allowed them to eat once they had escaped from the
puzzle box. Figure 9.5 shows some of the puzzle boxes he designed and built.
Years later, his son
---------------Insert
Figure 9.5 about here [Thorndike Puzzle Boxes]---------------
(R. Thorndike, 1991, p. 145) wrote
about his fatherÕs mechanical ability, Òif you look at pictures of the
equipment that he used in his original animal experiments you realize that they
would have shamed Rube Goldberg. [see below]Ó
Nevertheless and despite their
FYI: Rube Goldberg-Rube
Goldberg (1883-1970) was an American cartoonist who drew incredibly convoluted
and inane machines (see Figure 9.6) designed to accomplish simple tasks with
great difficulty.
---------------Insert
Figure 9.6 about here [Rube Goldberg back scratcher]---------------
primitiveness,
ThorndikeÕs puzzle boxes yielded extraordinary and original results. He noticed
that cats first attempted a large number of fruitless actions in their attempts
to escape. Gradually, those actions disappeared. Nearly always, cats made a
chance discovery of the action that operated the escape mechanism. When he
returned the cats that had discovered the trick for escaping from a particular
puzzle box, they exited the box more and more quickly. In other words, their latency
decreased. A cat that had been returned to the puzzle box many times escaped
almost as soon as it was put in.
From
his latency data, Thorndike constructed the first learning curves (see Figure
9.7). Those curves highlighted behaviors
Marginal Definition: learning curve-a
graphical representation of the progress of learning over time with the
dependent variable shown on the y-axis
and time shown on the x-axis.
and not
ideas as the basic data for psychology. Unwittingly perhaps,
Thorndike layed the groundwork for behaviorism,
psychologyÕs next school of thought (see chapter 10). He continued his animal experiments for a few years and
discovered that once a particular cat or dog learned to escape one of his
puzzle boxes they could again escape it even when placed there after a long
time. Furthermore, the behaviors that failed to lead to escape never returned.
He thought of the behaviors that led to escape as being Òstamped inÓ and those
that did not as being Òstamped out.Ó He also investigated whether his animals
could profit by instruction. They did not. He was surprised to discover that
his cats and dogs learned to escape no faster when their paws were moved by him
to make the requisite response. His major contributions however were his laws
of learning, the most important of which was his law of effect (Thorndike, 1911, p. 244):
Of several responses made to the same situation, those which
are accompanied or closely followed by satisfaction to the animal will, other
things being equal, be more firmly connected with the situation, so that, when
it recurs, they will be more likely to recur; those which are accompanied or
closely followed by discomfort to the animal will, other things being equal,
have their connections with that situation weakened, so that, when it recurs,
they will be less likely to occur. The greater the
satisfaction or discomfort, the greater the strengthening or weakening of the
bond.
Another
law Thorndike proposed was the law of
exercise. That law stated that learning would last longer when animals were
exposed to more instances of a stimulus (the puzzle box) and a response
(escaping). As Robinson (1981, p. 409) has pointed out, ÒThere is little in
either of these ÔlawsÕ that could not be gleaned from Locke, or Hume or Bentham
or, for that matter, Aristotle...The difference...is that the laws in
ThorndikeÕs case are supported by experimental findings.Ó Again, however,
Thorndike was anticipating behaviorism. His S-R (stimulus-response) connections
would become one of the mainstays of later behavioral psychology. When Cattell suggested that he begin to work with children,
Thorndike left animal work behind for over 30 years. His work with children led
to tremendous success in the applied areas of testing and educational
psychology.
FYI: Thorndike and monkey research-Thorndike
also worked with a number of monkeys. For them, he made the puzzle boxes so
they would have to open them in order to obtain some food. He discovered that
their learning curves were much steeper, meaning they learned faster. He
abandoned that line of research because of the difficulty of handling them; had
he continued he might have discovered that monkeys learn differently than do
cats and dogs.
After
he left Columbia as a student, he worked for a year at the College for Women of
Western Reserve College (now Case Western Reserve University) in Ohio. He
returned to Columbia, but not to the psychology department. Instead, he became
a member of their Teachers College, a position he held until he retired in 1939
(although he continue to work until his death ten years later). Thorndike and
Woodworth (1901) conducted an influential study on transfer of training from
one domain of human learning to another (e.g., from Latin to math). Their
results showed no such transfer took place and their work led to radical
revisions in school curricula. Similarly, Thorndike approached the design of
childrenÕs dictionaries. Before his analysis of them, those dictionaries were
simply abridged versions of adultÕs dictionaries. Working with Clarence
Barnhart, they created new childrenÕs dictionaries where the words used to
define terms were all simpler than the term itself and where pictures were used
extensively along with sentences designed to illustrate the meaning of terms.
Thorndike
also was one of the first successful developers of tests. He was so successful
that he was one of the first psychologists to make a substantial income from
his work outside of academe. He applied his statistical methods to vocational
guidance and academic success too. That work yielded low correlations for
workers followed over a nine-year period, but higher correlations for students
who reached college (R. Thorndike, 1991). Thorndike remained active until his
death in 1949. He lived to see psychology grow from its American infancy until
its post World War II boom years.